Cookies on this website

We use cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. If you click 'Accept all cookies' we'll assume that you are happy to receive all cookies and you won't see this message again. If you click 'Reject all non-essential cookies' only necessary cookies providing core functionality such as security, network management, and accessibility will be enabled. Click 'Find out more' for information on how to change your cookie settings.

EmoChi - Project

About

Humans are exceptionally social beings. Understanding others' emotions as well as regulating one’s own emotions are foundational skills that children acquire and continuously build across development to successfully navigate the social world. Emotions are an important element of nonverbal communication, in particular at an early age before children acquire the vocabulary to express how they themselves feel and to understand how others feel.

emotion

The goal of the EmoChi-project is to develop a knowledge-exchange hub to make research on emotional development accessible to children, parents, caretakers, and other stakeholders. We are looking to engage in a conversation about what it is we know about emotions in (early) childhood and how we can use this knowledge to improve children's social competence. This website is designed to help parents learn more about emotions, with the aim of encouraging meaningful family conversations and supporting children's emotional development.

The content of this website was created by the research team of the Social Foundations Lab at the University of Oxford and it is continuously updated as we incorporate feedback from parents and children.

News and updates

We have been working together with Gameloft and Ferrero Int. to integrate the content of this website within a game which introduces children to the topic emotions. The game follows an alien protagonist who wants to learn about human emotions.       

 

Alien_Pose_05.png QR_Code_final_center.png Alien_Pose_04.png B_Gameloft_for_brands_H_HD.png Alien_Pose_02.png
KinderKinderinilogowithoutline.jpg

 

The game is live on both the Google Play Store and the Apple App store. To explore the games you can scan the QR code above! We have co-designed the game with our partners to feature educational elements to encourage conversations between children and their parents about feelings and emotions in everyday life. 

What is an emotion

thumbnail_3_What_Is_An_Emotion.jpg

Emotions play a key role in our everyday lives. They influence how we interact with other people and how we navigate our (social) environment. For example, fear might prompt us to avoid danger, while happiness encourages social connection, and surprise might make us try and make sense of a situation. Despite their importance, and the attention they have received from different fields such as neuroscience, psychology and philosophy, it is difficult to define what an emotion is. Most researchers agree that emotions are complex psychological and physiological reactions to internal events (thoughts, imaginations) and external events (situations and context). Emotions can be characterised by:

  • Subjective experiences → The feelings we have when we experience an emotion.
  • Physiological responses → For example sweating, changes in heartbeat, and breathing which can activate or deactivate the body depending on the situation.
  • Behavioural responses → This can include facial expressions and bodily movements

emotion box

The development of emotions

Current research suggests that emotional understanding and perception develop significantly across childhood and into adulthood. Several factors have been found to play an important role. These include:

  • The family environment: For example, children who grow up in families that express emotions tend to develop better emotion understanding skills.
  • The child’s overall language development and conversations about emotions. For example, regular and open conversations about emotions within the family context have been found to enhance children’s emotional understanding.1

thumbnail_4_The_Development_Of_Emotions_.jpg

As parents it is important to recognise that there are important differences between adults’ and children's recognition and understanding of emotions. Studies indicate that children, in their preschool years, begin with two broad emotion categories - 'feels good' and 'feels bad'. With development and further experiences, children gradually acquire more specific emotion categories, and their understanding becomes more adult-like.2

emotion box 2

 

Within the Applaydu world, we focus on 4 groups of emotions that have been particularly researched from a developmental perspective. 

Kinderini_Experience_Menu.jpg

Positive emotions || Happiness

What is happiness?

Happiness, and joy, are considered positive emotions which are experienced as being pleasant and sometimes activating. Amongst the so-called universal emotions, happiness and joy are the most desirable. They do not only arise in response to doing or receiving something but also from a sense of connection between people or a sense of wellbeing.1

Children’s understanding of happiness develops gradually. By the age of five years, children understand what happiness means drawing from their own experiences of what felt ‘good’. Happiness was described as joy, love, feeling good, having fun, and just feeling really happy overall. Younger children find it difficult to explain the idea behind happiness. Children report feeling happy when they receive a new toy or play with friends. Explaining happiness for younger children is made easier by drawing on real-life examples.2

One study explored how parents communicate emotions to children aged 12 to 24 months during a shared book-reading task. Infants provided labels for emotions around the age of 18 months. Parents used more emotion labels for happiness and sadness, while fewer for disgust. This suggests parental communication influences children’s emotional understanding. Children may label happiness earlier due to parents' frequent discussion of this emotion, highlighting the significance of early emotional communication in development.3

To investigate how specific and general vocabulary influence emotion understanding, researchers presented children between the ages of 4 to 9 years with vignettes representing different emotions, including happiness, and asked children to indicate how the protagonist in the vignette felt. Children who knew more emotion specific words (beyond their general vocabulary size) demonstrated better emotion recognition and understanding of emotion regulation strategies. This suggests that such vocabulary extends beyond general language skills in enhancing emotional comprehension.4

How is happiness expressed?

The most common ways people describe the sensation of happiness is feeling light or uplifted, energetic, buzzing or tingling, warm, or grounded. The facial expression most commonly associated with happiness is a smile. We can change our facial expressions to look as though we are smiling (even if we do not experience happiness). There is a specific smile, the so-called Duchenne-smile which is difficult to stage and is therefore considered to be a genuine expression of happiness (including the characteristic ‘crow's feet’).

Happiness can be expressed in the tone of voice and in our posture: Some states of enjoyment may be signaled with a sigh of content, a squeal of joy, excited exclamation or shout, or laughter. Posture changes can be upright or elevated. Children show elevated posture after helping others and after seeing others being happy. For children at 5 years of age, this expression of happiness is stronger when others are watching their helping behaviour.5

thumbnail_6_How_Is_Happiness_Expressed.jpg

In one study, researchers focused on understanding children's happiness as a measure of their well-being. Happiness was explored in 6- to 8-year-olds through drawings, group interviews, and photos. Children drew, discussed, and took photos of things that made them happy. The themes which emerged revolved around instances of play and social interactions which significantly influenced children’s experience of happiness. By understanding what brings children joy, we gain insights into their overall well-being and can better support their happiness.6

What causes happiness?

Children’s relationship with their primary caregivers is a key factor influencing a child’s emotional development and thus their happiness. An example of this is the effect of caregiver’s emotion-based language. Parents more frequently use emotions labels for happiness compared to other emotions when describing pictures in a book to their children. This may explain why children are better at recognising happiness compared to other emotions.3 

Family dynamics and positive emotions go hand-in-hand. In one study including families from Mexico, a culture which is places a strong emphasis on community, found that mothers who shared positive emotions in their interactions with their child reported lower levels of parenting stress and they reported fewer temperamental outbursts from the children in their daily lives.7

thumbnail_7__What_Causes_Happiness.jpg

Researchers examined how caregivers’ emotions and behaviours during interactions with their preschool children (3- to 5-year-olds) impact children's overall happiness. Caregivers' own expression of happiness, rather than their approach behaviours, were linked to children's expressed positive emotions. However, specific caregiver actions such as narrating and sharing emotions with their child correlated positively with children's own happiness. This highlights the importance of caregivers being emotionally responsive to children because this shapes children's experiences and expressed happiness.8

Children express elevated happiness after sharing resources (such as cookies) with others. Five-year-old children were judged to look happier after sharing with others compared to when they themselves received a reward. This positive emotion was stronger after children themselves gave up a reward compared when then merely saw someone else receive a reward. In the literature this is referred to as the 'warm-glow effect'.9

In one study, young children felt happiest when playing with their favourite toys, when spending time with loved ones, getting hugs when they needed them, unwrapping presents, visiting exciting places, enjoying their favourite meals, feeling cozy and relaxed, and accomplishing something they worked hard for. The study concluded that it is the ‘simple’ things which often result in children feeling happy.2

One question researchers are interested in is how emotional expressivity and emotion regulation relate to children’s academic motivation, engagement, and overall achievement. Sixth graders (average age 10 years) who were motivated to do well at school and engagement were also judged to look happier. In particular hose children who were skilled at regulating their own emotions, negative and positive, showed higher academic motivation and higher academic achievement.10

drawinggroup.png

Good to know

Alongside age differences, there are also cultural influences. For example in western cultures, which tend to be individualistic, happiness reflects a high level of positive affect and more of a sense of satisfaction. In American English, the concept of happiness has evolved to denote positive internal feelings. Happiness is subjective, characterised by heightened positive emotions, diminished negative emotions, and possibly a feeling of contentment or fulfillment. In Mandarin Chinese there are different words to express different states of happiness, such as ‘Gao Xing’ for subjective feelings and ‘Kauai Le’ for general character.11

Awe is a positive emotion that, like surprise, arises from witnessing unexpected events. Similarly, it can elicit curiosity, encouraging learning, especially in young children.12 Awe has also been linked to prosocial behaviour, such as sharing and charity.13 The expression of awe includes widening of the eyes, raised inner eyebrow, open and slightly dropped jaw and mouth, goosebumps, small forward movement of the head, and visible inhalation.14

Pride is a positive emotion but also a self-reflective emotion, that occurs following a relevant accomplishment.15  Much research has investigated the development of pride in children, finding that pride can be recognised by children at the age of 4 years. The expression of pride includes a small smile, head titled slightly back, arms either raised above the head or placed on the hip, and a visibly expanded posture.16

Pride and happiness, though both positive emotions, can be distinguished by the fact that pride requires the individual to be and feel responsible for the positive event. Children progressively differentiate between these two emotions, a skill which matures between 9 and 11 years of age. Pride can also be derived from both self-achievement and altruistic behaviour (this is known as moral pride) and this form of pride develops later in development.17

In one study, researchers examined how different types of play, including pretend and physical play, predicted children's social and emotional skills over a period of two years. Younger children demonstrated lower emotional knowledge and regulation initially but improved over time. Sociodramatic play predicted better emotional expressiveness and regulation, while rough-and- tumble play predicted expressiveness and regulation, with gender differences observed in play preferences and emotional skills.18

thumbnail_8__Happiness__Good_To_Know.jpg

 

Here are examples of the texts on happiness we included in the Games: 

IMG_9428.PNG

IMG_9429.PNG

 

 

Negative Emotions || Fear

What is fear?

Fear, or fearfulness, is one of the so-called basic or universal emotions in response to perceived threats to our or others’ safety and well-fare. The experience and expression of fear is considered to serve important survival functions, for example to avoid physical or social harm but also to signal a need for help and to get attention from others.

thumbnail_9__What_Is_Fear.jpg

How is fear expressed?

We often experience fear when we evaluate events to be unexpected, unpleasant, caused by something outside our control, and if we find ourselves unable to cope in response to the actual or imagined threat.

Fear is said to have a characteristic facial expression which includes widened eyes, stretched lips, and raised eyebrows.1 However, there is considerable debate as to how universal (similar across cultures) these facial expressions are and how variable these expressions can be from person to person.

Experiencing fear is often described and characterised by heavy breathing, an increased heart rate, tightened neck muscles, increased pupil dilation, increased blood pressure, as well as increased skin conductance, e.g., ‘sweaty palms’.2 Fearfulness can also be expressed in the tone of voice to include screams and gasps.3,4,5

thumbnail_10_How_Is_Fear_Expressed.jpg

What causes fear?

We can classify different types of fear: arising from social interactions or from shared environments, in response to actual or perceived harm, triggered by actual (or imagined) animals, or resulting from being in crowed spaces (i.e., agoraphobia).6

Research found that the majority infants exhibit fear of strangers as they reach the end of their first year of life.7

According to evolutionary psychology, experiencing and expressing fear is adaptive and increases an individual’s chances of survival. However, most research seems to suggest that we are not born with innate fear responses. Instead, it is suggested that human are born with the capacity (including attentional biases) to develop fears of certain events.8

thumbnail_11_What_Causes_Fear.jpg

Good to know

Research on fear regulation has highlighted the advantage of attentional redirection. For example, children who were scared of blood being drawn using needles at the doctors, show reduced fear when distracted by hand puppets.9 Caregivers can help reduce fear in children by redirecting their attention from the fear-evoking stimulus onto a comforting object or toy.

Studies found that children pay special attention to caregivers and modify their fear responses accordingly. When caregivers express fear and/or anxious behaviour, children mirror that response to that stimulus.10 Similarly, children also pay attention to their caregiver’s protective behaviour, causing them to remain in a state of heightened alert.11

In an experimental paradigm where infants can cross a visual (non-real) cliff, it was found infants’ approach or avoidance behaviour on the visual cliff was influenced by their mothers’ emotional facial expressions.12

Though there is some debate about the specific brain regions involved in fear processing, much research suggests primarily activation in the amygdala, as well as other regions such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the putamen.13

thumbnail_12__Fear__Good_To_Know_.jpg

 

 

Here is an example of the information on fear we included in the Games: 

 

 

Prosocial Emotions || kindness

What is Kindness?

Kindness describes the many behaviours and emotions that motivate people to help, comfort, and share with others. There is no single emotion of ‘kindness’ but we consider kind behaviours to be motivated by emotions which are felt in concern for others’ well-being. We feel empathy when we share the emotions of others and sympathy when we feel concern for others in need (instead of mirroring their emotions).

thumbnail_13__What_Is_Kindness.jpg

How is kindness expressed?

Feeling sympathy and concern for others has a distinct facial expression which is characterised by furrowed or raised eyebrows and a sad expression in the eyes. In addition, feeling sympathy and concern for others is often accompanied by a slowing of our heart rate which is a sign of increased attention to others (and their needs).

Children show concern for others from as early as 10 months of age. The extent to which children express concern is related to whether and how much children help others. Interestingly, it is the expression of concern and not mere attention to others which is related to children’s helping behaviour.1 

Children can show concern in a variety of ways. Beyond expressing an emotion, feeling concern can be expressed in a variety of helping behaviours including handing others out-of-reach objects, comforting those who are hurt, and sharing resources with others.2 

We refer to helping as being motivated by kindness if it is accompanied by genuine concern for others’ well-being (as opposed to more selfish motivations). Kindness refers to motivations as much as emotions and children as young as 2 years show positive emotions if others’ needs are fulfilled.3,4

thumbnail_14_How_Is_Kindness_Expressed.jpg

What causes kindness?

We feel sympathy when we see others being hurt or when others are struggling or if they are in need of help. The feelings of kindness are about other people and not so much about ourselves. Being kind means that we want to make others feel better.

Kindness, and helping behaviour, are as much a part of human nature (our biology) as they are part of human culture (our upbringing and shared values). There is extensive research on the genetic basis of children’s concern for others5 but there is an equal amount of research on the influence on sociialization on children’s kindness.6 

Kindness does not have a single cause but it is best described by emotions and motivations that are directed toward others’ needs. Children from as young as 9 months of age understand others’ needs, that is they can anticipate how others are best helped and it is both the understanding of others’ needs as well as children’s motor abilities which predicts children’s helping behaviour.7

thumbnail_15__What_Causes_Kindness.jpg

Good to know

There are many good reasons to be kind to others. Not only does it motivate us to make others feel better, but it can also make ourselves feel better about having acted kindly. In addition, our friends and families value kind behaviour and being kind can help us to make friends.

Comparative work suggests that some of the emotions and motivations underlying young children’s helping behaviour are shared with other primates. For example, chimpanzees help others in similar ways as do young children i.e., by providing access to food or by removing obstacles.8

Studies on children’s helping behaviour often include clever paradigms to elicit helping and kindness in children. Examples include an adult, either the parent or another adult, accidentally bumping their knee on a table and subsequently expressing mild pain.9 In other studies, the adult pretends to be clumsy and accidentally drops objects or misplaces them.10

There is a lively debate in developmental science on how children’s kindness changes as they grow older. One milestone development is perspective taking and concern for one’s reputation. Children older than 4 years of age care about whether they are being watched while helping others, presumably to manage their good reputation in the eye of onlookers.11

thumbnail_16__Kindness__Good_To_Know.jpg

Learning & Curiosity || Surprise 

What is surprise?

Surprise is an emotion that arises from a detected mismatch between expectation and experience.1,2 The emotion is characterised by a sense of difficulty to explain how or why something happened, and is believed to motivate us to seek explanations and to make sense of the world.3,4

What is the role of expectations and predictions for our lives? In recent years, findings suggest that the human brain is not entirely reliant upon external information (through vision, smell, touch etc.). Instead, memories of previous experiences and statistical learning are used to produce predictions about current and future states.

thumbnail_17__What_Is_Surprise_.jpg

How is surprise expressed?

Surprise is assumed to be typically expressed by raised eyebrows, an open mouth, a gasp or intake of air, as well as dilation of the pupils.5 From an evolutionary point of view, this behaviour is assumed to maximise the intake of information from the environment.

Research investigating young children’s ability to discriminate between different emotions has found that, for example, whilst happiness and sadness can be distinguished from a very early age, surprise recognition develops slightly later.6 Interestingly, fear and surprise expressions are often difficult to differentiate, which may be related to the number of similarities shared by facial patterns of the two emotions, including the raised inner and outer brow, the raised upper eyelid, and the open mouth.7

Surprise expressions develop gradually with age – whilst only 30% of 3.5-month-old infants show surprise, roughly 67% of 5.5-month-old infants display surprise. This may be encouraged by caregivers’ interactions with their children as some research has shown that mothers display exaggerated surprise expressions towards their infant.8

Cultural differences have been shown to exist for surprise causes and responses. For example, caretakers in East Asia have been found to express less surprise than people from Western cultures.9 This may be due to social norms, as researchers found that when the East Asian participants were asked to display less emotional control, they showed surprise responses similar to the American participants.10

thumbnail_18__How_Is_Surprise_Expressed_.jpg

What causes surprise?

Surprise is triggered by unexpected events. This can be for example hearing a loud sound suddenly next to you or seeing a person you know in a place you would not think they would be at. The key aspect here is the unexpectedness, as you cannot be surprised by something you know is going to happen.

thumbnail_19__What_Causes_Surprise.jpg

Research often use surprise or surprise responses such as longer looking times or dilated pupils to measure what children know about the world. For example, experiments have been done which looked at children’s development of knowledge about intuitive physics (how do two objects behave when they clash with each other) or measuring if children form expectations based on probability.11

Good to know: Surprise and Learning

Surprise is an emotion that has been linked to exploration and learning.12 Research suggests a chain reaction in which a person’s expectations are defied, resulting in surprise, which leads to curiosity/confusion/interest, then exploration of the stimulus that triggered this process, which finally results in learning.13

Research has found that ‘desirable difficulties’, challenges that push children to seek knowledge, lead to more curious behaviour which encourages memory and learning.14 However, there is a possibility that presenting children with too much new and surprising information can cause children to feel overwhelmed, anxious and confused – as with everything, balance is important!

thumbnail_20__Surprise__Good_To_Know.jpg

 

Here is an example of the texts on surprise we included in the Games: 

IMG_9427.PNG

 

Research Team 

Robert Hepach - Associate Professor of Developmental Psychology 

Principal Investigator 

Robert Hepach

Clemens Von Wulffen - DPhil Candidate

Doctoral student

Clemens v. Wulffen

Mary Astor

Research Assistant

Mary Astor

Zuha Murtaza

Research Intern 

Zuha Murtaza

Ranya Hossain

Research Intern 

Ranya Hossain

Diana Signer

Research Intern 

Diana Signer 

Charlotte Dale

Research Intern

Charlotte Dale 

 

Artwork 

Illustrations provided by Allysa Adams

 www.allysaadamsillustration.com

References 

Development of emotions 

  1. Ogren, Marissa, and Scott P. Johnson. "Factors facilitating early emotion understanding development: Contributions to individual differences." Human development 64, no. 3 (2021): 108-118.
  2. Widen, Sherri C. "Children’s interpretation of facial expressions: The long path from valence-based to specific discrete categories." Emotion Review 5, no. 1 (2013): 72-77.

Happiness 

  1. Messinger, Daniel, Samantha G. Mitsven, Yeojin Amy Ahn, Emily B. Prince, Lei Sun, and Camila Rivero-Fernández. "Happiness and joy." Handbook of emotional development (2019): 171-198.
  2. Hong, Yonghee, Younge Ra, and Hyunjin Jang. "A Study on Young Children's Perceptions and Experiences of Happiness." Asia-Pacific Journal of Research in Early Childhood Education 9, no. 1 (2015).
  3. Ruba, Ashley L., Seth D. Pollak, and Jenny R. Saffran. "Acquiring complex communicative systems: Statistical learning of language and emotion." Topics in Cognitive Science 14, no. 3 (2022): 432-450.
  4. Streubel, Berit, Catherine Gunzenhauser, Gerlind Grosse, and Henrik Saalbach. "Emotion-specific vocabulary and its contribution to emotion understanding in 4-to 9-year-old children." Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 193 (2020): 104790.
  5. Hepach, Robert, Jan M. Engelmann, Esther Herrmann, Stella C. Gerdemann, and Michael Tomasello. "Evidence for a developmental shift in the motivation underlying helping in early childhood." Developmental Science 26, no. 1 (2023): e13253.
  6. Moore, Alice, and Helen Lynch. "Understanding a child’s conceptualisation of well-being through an exploration of happiness: The centrality of play, people and place." Journal of Occupational Science 25, no. 1 (2018): 124-141.
  7. Lindsey, Eric W. "Shared positive emotion during parent-toddler play and parent and child well-being in Mexican origin families." Infant Behavior and Development 67 (2022): 101706.
  8. Hill, Taylor G., Emma C. Coughlan, and Sean P. Mackinnon. "Crafting happiness from everyday life: personality, personal projects, basic psychological need satisfaction, and well-being." International Journal of Personality Psychology 9 (2023): 47-60.
  9. Aknin, Lara B., J. Kiley Hamlin, and Elizabeth W. Dunn. "Giving leads to happiness in young children." PLoS one 7, no. 6 (2012): e39211.
  10. Kwon, Kyongboon, Amanda R. Hanrahan, and Kevin A. Kupzyk. "Emotional expressivity and emotion regulation: Relation to academic functioning among elementary school children." School Psychology Quarterly 32, no. 1 (2017): 75.
  11. Yang, Fan, Joshua Knobe, and Yarrow Dunham. "Happiness is from the soul: The nature and origins of our happiness concept." Journal of experimental psychology: General 150, no. 2 (2021): 276.
  12. Colantonio II, Joseph A., and Elizabeth Bonawitz. "Awesome play: Awe increases preschooler’s exploration and discovery." In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, vol. 40. 2018.
  13. Stamkou, Eftychia, Eddie Brummelman, Rohan Dunham, Milica Nikolic, and Dacher Keltner. "Awe sparks prosociality in children." Psychological science 34, no. 4 (2023): 455-467.
  14. Lucht, Antonia, and Hein T. van Schie. "The evolutionary function of awe: a review and integrated model of seven theoretical perspectives." Emotion Review 16, no. 1 (2024): 46-63.
  15. Peplak, Joanna, Beatrice Bobba, Mari Hasegawa, Simona Caravita, and Tina Malti. "The warm glow of kindness: Developmental insight into children’s moral pride across cultures and its associations with prosocial behavior." Developmental psychology (2023).
  16. Tracy, Jessica L., Richard W. Robins, and Kristin H. Lagattuta. "Can children recognize pride?." Emotion 5, no. 3 (2005): 251.
  17. Kornilaki, Ekaterina N., and Gregory Chlouverakis. "The situational antecedents of pride and happiness: Developmental and domain differences." British Journal of Developmental Psychology 22, no. 4 (2004): 605-619.
  18. Lindsey, Eric W., and Malinda J. Colwell. "Pretend and physical play: Links to preschoolers' affective social competence." Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 59, no. 3 (2013): 330-360.

Fear 

  1. Ekman, Paul. "Darwin, deception, and facial expression." Annals of the new York Academy of sciences 1000, no. 1 (2003): 205-221.
  2. Kreibig, Sylvia D. "Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review." Biological psychology 84, no. 3 (2010): 394-421.
  3. Barrett, Lisa Feldman. "The conceptual act theory: A précis." Emotion review 6, no. 4 (2014): 292-297.
  4. Sauter, Disa A., Frank Eisner, Paul Ekman, and Sophie K. Scott. "Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, no. 6 (2010): 2408-2412.
  5. Keltner, Dacher, Jessica Tracy, Disa A. Sauter, Daniel C. Cordaro, and Galen McNeil. "Expression of emotion." Handbook of emotions 4 (2016): 467-482.
  6. Arrindell, Willem A., Mary J. Pickersgill, Harald Merckelbach, Angélique M. Ardon, and Frieda C. Cornet. "Phobic dimensions: III. Factor analytic approaches to the study of common phobic fears; an updated review of findings obtained with adult subjects." Advances in behaviour research and therapy 13, no. 2 (1991): 73-130.
  7. Brooks, Jeanne, and Michael Lewis. "Attachment behavior in thirteen-month-old, opposite-sex twins." Child Development (1974): 243-247.
  8. Thrasher, Cat, and Vanessa LoBue. "Do infants find snakes aversive? Infants’ physiological responses to “fear-relevant” stimuli." Journal of experimental child psychology 142 (2016): 382-390.
  9. Oluç, Tuğba, and Arzu Sarialioğlu. "The effect of a hand puppet-based therapeutic play for preschool children on the fear and pain associated with blood collection procedure." Journal of Pediatric Nursing 72 (2023): e80-e86.
  10. De Rosnay, Marc, Peter J. Cooper, Nicolas Tsigaras, and Lynne Murray. "Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm." Behaviour research and therapy 44, no. 8 (2006): 1165-1175.
  11. Buss, Kristin A., and Elizabeth J. Kiel. "Do maternal protective behaviors alleviate toddlers’ fearful distress?." International Journal of Behavioral Development 35, no. 2 (2011): 136-143.
  12. Klinnert, Mary D., Joseph J. Campos, James F. Sorce, Robert N. Emde, and M. A. R. I. L. Y. N. Svejda. "Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy." In Emotions in early development, pp. 57-86. Academic Press, 1983.
  13. Thomas, Kathleen M., Wayne C. Drevets, Ronald E. Dahl, Neal D. Ryan, Boris Birmaher, Clayton H. Eccard, David Axelson, Paul J. Whalen, and B. J. Casey. "Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children." Archives of general psychiatry 58, no. 11 (2001): 1057-1063.

Kindness

  1. Roth-Hanania, Ronit, Maayan Davidov, and Carolyn Zahn-Waxler. "Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others." Infant Behavior and Development 34, no. 3 (2011): 447-458.
  2. Dunfield, Kristen A. "A construct divided: Prosocial behavior as helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes." Frontiers in psychology 5 (2014): 958.
  3. Aknin, Lara B., J. Kiley Hamlin, and Elizabeth W. Dunn. "Giving leads to happiness in young children." PLoS one 7, no. 6 (2012): e39211.
  4. Hepach, Robert, Amrisha Vaish, and Michael Tomasello. "The fulfillment of others’ needs elevates children’s body posture." Developmental Psychology 53, no. 1 (2017): 100.
  5. Zahn-Waxler, Carolyn, Marian Radke-Yarrow, Elizabeth Wagner, and Michael Chapman. "Development of concern for others." Developmental psychology 28, no. 1 (1992): 126.
  6. Brownell, Celia A., and Early Social Development Research Lab. "Prosocial behavior in infancy: The role of socialization." Child Development Perspectives 10, no. 4 (2016): 222-227.
  7. Köster, Moritz, Xenia Ohmer, Thanh Dung Nguyen, and Joscha Kärtner. "Infants understand others’ needs." Psychological science 27, no. 4 (2016): 542-548.
  8. Köster, Moritz, Shoji Itakura, Masaki Omori, and Joscha Kärtner. "From understanding others' needs to prosocial action: Motor and social abilities promote infants' helping." Developmental science 22, no. 6 (2019): e12804.
  9. Melis, Alicia P. "The evolutionary roots of prosociality: the case of instrumental helping." Current Opinion in Psychology 20 (2018): 82-86.
  10. Warneken, Felix, and Michael Tomasello. "Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees." science 311, no. 5765 (2006): 1301-1303.
  11. Engelmann, Jan M., and Diotima J. Rapp. "The influence of reputational concerns on children's prosociality." Current Opinion in Psychology 20 (2018): 92-95.

Surprise 

  1. Barto, Andrew, Marco Mirolli, and Gianluca Baldassarre. "Novelty or surprise?." Frontiers in psychology 4 (2013): 907.
  2. Ekman, Paul. "Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: a reply to Russell's mistaken critique." (1994): 268.
  3. Foster, Meadhbh I., and Mark T. Keane. "Why some surprises are more surprising than others: Surprise as a metacognitive sense of explanatory difficulty." Cognitive psychology 81 (2015): 74-116.
  4. Maguire, Rebecca, Phil Maguire, and Mark T. Keane. "Making sense of surprise: an investigation of the factors influencing surprise judgments." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37, no. 1 (2011): 176.
  5. Ekman, Paul. "Darwin, deception, and facial expression." Annals of the new York Academy of sciences 1000, no. 1 (2003): 205-221.
  6. Widen, Sherri C. "Children’s interpretation of facial expressions: The long path from valence-based to specific discrete categories." Emotion Review 5, no. 1 (2013): 72-77.
  7. Jack, Rachael E., Wei Sun, Ioannis Delis, Oliver GB Garrod, and Philippe G. Schyns. "Four not six: Revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145, no. 6 (2016): 708.
  8. Reissland, Nadja, John Shepherd, and Lily Cowie. "The melody of surprise: Maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant." Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice 11, no. 3 (2002): 271-278.
  9. Choi, Incheol, and Richard E. Nisbett. "Cultural psychology of surprise: holistic theories and recognition of contradiction." Journal of personality and social psychology 79, no. 6 (2000): 890.
  10. Valenzuela, Ana, Barbara Mellers, and Judi Strebel. "Pleasurable surprises: A cross-cultural study of consumer responses to unexpected incentives." Journal of Consumer Research 36, no. 5 (2010): 792-805.
  11. Doan, Tiffany, Ori Friedman, and Stephanie Denison. "Beyond belief: The probability-based notion of surprise in children." Emotion 18, no. 8 (2018): 1163.
  12. Wentworth, Naomi, and Sam L. Witryol. "What's new? Three dimensions for defining novelty." The Journal of genetic psychology 147, no. 2 (1986): 209-218.
  13. Stahl, Aimee E., and Lisa Feigenson. "Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration." Science 348, no. 6230 (2015): 91-94.
  14. Fandakova, Yana, and Matthias J. Gruber. "States of curiosity and interest enhance memory differently in adolescents and in children." Developmental Science 24, no. 1 (2021): e13005.